Why is it surprisingly difficult to successfully tickle yourself
Ever wonder why someone else's touch can send you into fits of laughter, yet your own tickle attempts leave you cold? The answer lies in a fascinating trick your brain plays—find out what it is.


Too Long; Didn't Read
Tickling requires surprise; your brain predicts your own touch, cancelling the ticklish feeling when you try it on yourself.
The Un-Tickleable You: Why is it Surprisingly Difficult to Successfully Tickle Yourself?
Go ahead, try it. Wiggle your fingers under your own armpit or across the soles of your feet. Chances are, you'll feel the touch, but that uncontrollable, squirming, laugh-inducing tickle? It’s likely completely absent. This common experience raises a fascinating question: Why is it surprisingly difficult to successfully tickle yourself, even when you know exactly where your most ticklish spots are? It seems counterintuitive, but the answer lies deep within the predictive power of your own brain. This post delves into the neuroscience behind why self-tickling just doesn't work.
The inability to tickle ourselves isn't a flaw; it's a feature of how our brains process sensory information, particularly touch. It highlights a fundamental aspect of self-awareness and the brain's remarkable ability to distinguish between sensations caused by our own actions and those originating from the outside world. Understanding this phenomenon unlocks insights into sensory perception and motor control.
The Tickle Sensation: More Than Just Touch
Before diving into why self-tickling fails, let's understand what tickling is. Scientists often distinguish between two types:
- Knismesis: A light, feather-like touch that might cause an itching sensation or goosebumps, but usually not laughter. Think of a fly landing on your arm.
- Gargalesis: The heavier, laughter-inducing type of tickling, typically applied to specific body areas like the ribs, feet, or underarms. This is the kind we're focused on when asking why we can't do it to ourselves.
Gargalesis triggers a complex response involving not just the sense of touch but also areas of the brain associated with pleasure, emotion (like the anterior cingulate cortex), and the fight-or-flight response. The laughter, often involuntary, isn't always purely joyful; it can feel frantic or even uncomfortable. Some researchers theorize it might have evolved as a social bonding mechanism or even a defense reflex training tool in early life. Crucially, the element of surprise seems vital for this response.
Your Brain's Built-In Spoiler Alert: The Cerebellum
The main reason you can't tickle yourself lies in a part of your brain located at the back, beneath the main cerebrum: the cerebellum. While often associated with balance, coordination, and fine motor control, the cerebellum also plays a critical role as a prediction machine.
Here's how it works in the context of self-tickling:
- Action Planning: When you decide to move your fingers to tickle your ribs, your brain generates motor commands.
- Prediction: Simultaneously, the cerebellum predicts the sensory consequences of that planned movement. It essentially tells the rest of the brain, "Alert: Fingers are about to touch the ribs. Expected sensation incoming."
- Sensory Dampening: Because the cerebellum accurately predicts the timing and specific feel of the touch, it sends signals that dampen or effectively cancel out the sensory input received by other brain regions, particularly the somatosensory cortex (which processes touch) and the anterior cingulate cortex (involved in the emotional/unpleasant aspect of tickling).
This predictive cancellation prevents the sensation from being registered as unexpected or external. Without the element of surprise, the tickle response simply isn't triggered. Think of it as your brain issuing a spoiler alert for its own actions.
The Importance of the Unexpected
When someone else tickles you, your cerebellum cannot accurately predict the precise timing, pressure, and location of their touch. Even if you see them coming, the exact sensory input is unknown. This lack of prediction means the sensory signals travel unimpeded to the relevant brain areas, triggering the full, surprising, and often overwhelming tickle response.
- Evidence from Research: Studies, notably those led by neuroscientist Sarah-Jayne Blakemore at University College London, have provided strong evidence for this cerebellar hypothesis. Using fMRI brain scans, researchers observed significantly less activity in the somatosensory cortex and anterior cingulate cortex when participants attempted to tickle themselves compared to when they were tickled by a researcher or even a robot controlled unpredictably by the researcher. When the robot's touch was directly controlled by the participant (making it predictable), the tickle response was again diminished, mirroring self-tickling.
Rare Exceptions and What They Tell Us
While the vast majority of people cannot tickle themselves, some research suggests that individuals with certain neurological conditions, such as schizophrenia, might experience a reduced ability to distinguish between self-generated actions and external ones. This breakdown in the self-monitoring mechanism, potentially linked to cerebellar function, could theoretically allow for some degree of self-tickling in rare cases, though this remains an area needing more investigation. These exceptions further underscore the importance of the brain's predictive mechanism in the typical inability to self-tickle.
Conclusion: Your Predictive Brain at Work
So, why is it surprisingly difficult to successfully tickle yourself? The answer lies not in your technique, but in the sophisticated predictive power of your cerebellum. By anticipating the sensory consequences of your own movements, your brain effectively cancels out the element of surprise necessary to trigger the gargalesis tickle response. This mechanism is fundamental, distinguishing self-caused sensations from external ones, preventing us from being constantly overwhelmed by the feelings generated by our own bodies moving through the world.
While seemingly a trivial phenomenon, the inability to self-tickle offers a window into the intricate workings of sensory processing, motor control, and self-awareness within the human brain. It’s a simple, everyday demonstration of just how adept our brains are at understanding the difference between "me" and "not me."
More Articles

How can a dead body sometimes get goosebumps?
It’s a chilling sight that blurs the line between life and death—a corpse covered in goosebumps. Uncover the bizarre scientific reason for this strange post-mortem phenomenon.

What is the secret code hidden in the color of your bread bag's plastic clip?
That colorful plastic clip on your bread bag isn't a random choice; it's a secret baker's code that reveals the freshest loaf on the shelf.

Why do small floating things like cereal pieces clump together in a bowl of milk?
Ever wondered about the invisible force that herds your last few cereal pieces into a floating clump? It’s not a breakfast conspiracy, but a captivating lesson in surface tension happening right in your bowl.